Max 72% OFF QDY -Chimney Cleaning Brush Kit Chimney Sweeping S Sweep $47 QDY -Chimney Cleaning Brush Kit, Chimney Sweeping Brush, Sweep S Home Kitchen Heating, Cooling Air Quality Parts Accessories $47 QDY -Chimney Cleaning Brush Kit, Chimney Sweeping Brush, Sweep S Home Kitchen Heating, Cooling Air Quality Parts Accessories Chimney,/cachucha885332.html,Brush,,Home Kitchen , Heating, Cooling Air Quality , Parts Accessories,-Chimney,gateopenerus.com,Brush,Sweep,$47,Kit,,Cleaning,S,Sweeping,QDY Chimney,/cachucha885332.html,Brush,,Home Kitchen , Heating, Cooling Air Quality , Parts Accessories,-Chimney,gateopenerus.com,Brush,Sweep,$47,Kit,,Cleaning,S,Sweeping,QDY Max 72% OFF QDY -Chimney Cleaning Brush Kit Chimney Sweeping S Sweep
QDY -Chimney Cleaning Brush Kit, Chimney Sweeping Brush, Sweep S
$47
QDY -Chimney Cleaning Brush Kit, Chimney Sweeping Brush, Sweep S
This fits your .
Make sure this fits
by entering your model number.
REINFORCED POLYESTER FIBER HEAD: The pole and working head are made of reinforced Polyester fiber, which is durable and will not scratch the inner surface of the chimney flue. Suitable for metal flues and brick chimneys of different diameters and shapes.
PROFESSIONALLY USED: The creosote and other debris in the chimney can interfere with proper ventilation and have potential risks. This chimney brush can efficiently remove oil stains in the chimney that may cause a fire, making the use of the chimney safer.
EASY TO CLEAN: According to the actual selection of the corresponding pole length, the chimney can be efficiently cleaned from the bottom to the top. According to the actual selection of the corresponding pole length.
CONVENIENT AND QUICK INSTALLATION: Easy to use and operate, connect multiple rods to expand coverage. The rod is flexible and can bend perfectly through those difficult flues. Suitable for metal flues and brick chimneys of different diameters and shapes.
WIDE APPLICATION: It can be used with electric drills (the package does not include electric drills). It is suitable for metal flues and brick chimneys of different diameters and shapes. It has a replacement brush head and a hand-held brush head.
|||
Product description
Size:410mm
Chimney Cleaner Brush Clean Rotary Sweep System Fireplace Kit Rod Tool Set home kichen cleaning brush
Feature: - Suitable for metal flues and brick chimneys of different diameters and shapes. - According to the actual selection of the corresponding pole length. - Able to clean the chimney efficiently from bottom to top. - Convenient quick disassembly and assembly structure, easy to use and operate. - Will not scratch the inner surface of the pipe. - The working head is made of reinforced polyester fiber, which is wear-resistant, has a long life and is not easy to break.
Specification: Material: Polyester fiber Size: Polyester fiber rod length is about 410mm/16.14inch, 610mm/24inch
Packing List: 410mm*100mm*9 Rod + 1*Brush Head + 1*Chuck Adapter + 1*Small Wrench
Warm prompt: 1. Only the above list is included. The other products in the picture only show how they are used. 2. As different measuring methods,it will occur 1-3 cm deviation about the items,please ensure that it is okay for you before ordering. 3. Because of different monitors and lamplight,the picture may not reflect the actual color of the item,really thank you for your kind understanding!
QDY -Chimney Cleaning Brush Kit, Chimney Sweeping Brush, Sweep S
A new study, published in Psychology of Violence, looks at rates of intimate partner violence during the pandemic in the United States. Like data from the UK, it suggests that domestic violence increased during lockdown — and that this was particularly linked to stress.
What is it that makes someone feel that theirs is a “good life”? Of all the ideas put forward over the past few millennia, two are most often extolled and researched today. The first is hedonistic wellbeing, often called simply “happiness”, which is characterised by plenty of positive emotions and general life satisfaction. The other is “eudaimonia” — feeling that your life has meaning and that you are realising your potential. Now in a new paper in Psychological Review, Shigehiro Oishi at the University of Virginia and Erin Westgate at the University of Florida suggest that we’ve been missing something: “psychological richness”.
When thinking about homelessness, we don’t often consider where to draw the line between housed and homeless. Couchsurfers — homeless individuals who put a roof over their head by staying with friends, relatives, or strangers found on couchsurfing sites — may not spring to mind when considering homelessness.
However, it’s far from a rare arrangement. Though exact numbers are lacking, studies from the last five years found that a shocking 22% of young people in the UK had slept rough at some point, and that 35% had couchsurfed in the absence of having a stable home.
The lack of stability, security, and sense of belonging that comes with having a home are all recognised factors in adverse psychological outcomes in those who are homeless. But, with couchsurfing being such a prevalent living situation, yet so different from sleeping rough, the psychological effects of this specific type of homelessness are well worth investigating. Now a new study from researchers led by Katie Hail-Jares at Griffith University, Australia has uncovered a strong relationship between couchsurfing and psychological distress.
Much has been written about the downsides of home-working. “Zoom fatigue”, in particular, is now a term, and an experience, that many of us are familiar with. But the tiring effect of video chat could represent only one of its dangers, according to new work in PNAS. It finds that we ascribe less “mind” to people we see in image form, vs in the flesh, and even less again to images of images of people. There could be serious implications, write Paris Will at the University of British Columbia and colleagues: “Given that mind perception underpins moral judgement, our findings suggest that depicted persons will receive greater or lesser ethical consideration, depending on the level of abstraction.”
Around one in five of those who have recovered from Covid-19 report ongoing symptoms, also known as long Covid. Experiences with this new condition are varied, and several symptoms are neuropsychological in nature.
One such symptom is brain fog. Though not a medical diagnosis in itself, this term is recognised by many health professionals, and refers to a fluctuating and varied set of symptoms which severely affect the sufferer’s ability to think clearly, or conduct their lives as they previously have.
Brain fog is often thought of as a benign, non-specific symptom, and in some circles is even dismissed as malingering. But in fact, it’s a symptom widely associated with chemotherapy, an issue for 40% of those with HIV, and source of frustration for many during pregnancy, amongst other medical conditions. Several neurological mechanisms have been proposed, but as of yet scientists don’t agree on the exact physical cause. As such, research looking into this after-effect of Covid is likely to garner a wide array of responses.
At this stage, understanding the experience of brain fog in long Covid is important — in order to tackle a new condition, researchers must first obtain a thorough description of the problem. This is the starting point from which further research can truly begin. To this end, researchers based at Oxford University recruited 50 participants from previous long Covid studies and online long Covid support groups to participate in remotely-held focus groups.
The media plays a huge part in shaping our understanding of the world, including how we respond to other people. Coverage of immigration is no different, and previous research has suggested that even subtle changes in language and framing can change the way people think about immigrants.
A new study, published in Scientific Reports, looks at the real life impact of negative media portrayals of immigrants. It finds that negative coverage can increase hostility towards immigrants and favouritism towards members of the non-immigrant in-group — which can have serious financial, emotional and social consequences for communities.
Retaining new information can be tricky, especially with topics far outside of what we’re familiar with. A good teacher can make a huge difference, but effective teaching techniques can add new dimensions to our ability to really take on what we’re being told.
A new study by academics from the University of California and University of Georgia identifies one such technique, and it turns out to be incredibly simple: hand gestures.
Research has found significant racial biases when judging the emotions of others. Black people are more likely to be misjudged as angry, for example, and recent research has suggested that even children are victims of this “anger bias”. Black children are also frequently subject to “adultification” — being perceived as older and more mature than White peers.
A new study explores the links between these two phenomena, finding that the older adults believe Black children to be, the more likely they are to (incorrectly) judge them to be angry too. Writing in Cognition and Emotion, Alison N. Cooke and Amy G. Halberstadt from North Carolina State University argue that such judgements could have serious consequences for Black children.
It’s no secret that marginalised groups face barriers in educational settings that the able-bodied, male, and racially privileged largely do not. Issues pertaining to access, sense of belonging, potential discrimination, and financial difficulties can add often insurmountable layers of complexity to applying for further education.
Efforts to address this in recent years have crystallised into a number of measures, including the wide adoption of diversity and inclusion grants. These provide financial support specifically for selected individuals from marginalised groups, in order to give them a more equal footing with applicants from privileged backgrounds.
However, recent research from Adriana L. Germano and colleagues at The University of Washington has illustrated some unintended flaws with this approach. In stark contrast with the ethos behind diversity and inclusion initiatives, these grants may be serving to make applications to prestigious scholarships even less diverse than before.
In 1995, a seminal book was published suggesting that children from lower socioeconomic backgrounds were exposed to 30 million fewer words than richer children by the age of 4 — the so-called “word gap”. The idea is now widespread and has informed early childhood policy in the United States (though the findings are more contentious than this ubiquity might suggest).
But why might these kids be exposed to fewer words? A new study from a team at the University of California, Berkeley, finds that worries about financial insecurity reduced the amount that caregivers spoke to their small children, suggesting that these concerns themselves could be at least partly responsible for the word gap.